Other Animals
African grey parrot
Photo by Kevin Tribelhorn on Unsplash

A journal article, published this August (Schnell et al., 2021), showed that a species of European jay demonstrates clear distress when their expectations of finding a treat are violated. The experimenters showed the birds that a very desirable food was being hidden under one of two cups, and then, without the birds knowing, the experimenters swapped it for something the birds didn’t like nearly as much. Not all birds reacted strongly, but those that did check under other cups, or squawked and flew away, and usually ignored the less-desired food they had found.

Parrots Dislike Trickery Too!

I wasn’t at all surprised by their results, having observed the same behavior in my parrots (Pepperberg et al., 1997) decades ago! We performed the study as part of an experiment on object permanence—the notion that an object does not cease to exist (is ‘permanent’) even if it is out of sight. The very first studies of this type were performed by a Swiss psychologist, Jean Piaget, who was interested in studying the development of various types of intelligence in children. He started testing babies when they were only a few months old and kept presenting more and more complicated types of tasks as the children got older (Piaget, 1936/1952).

The easiest task just checked to see if the baby could track the object as it was moved around; then the tasks got harder: The object was partially hidden under something like a blanket, then fully hidden; next came tracking when two possible hiding places existed, then three, and so forth. Other researchers (e.g., Uzgaris & Hunt, 1975) looked at even more complicated tasks—actions like hiding the treat in the experimenter’s hand and then moving the hand among several cups before hiding the treat under one cup and showing an empty hand.

When we did the study in question, Alex had already passed all the Uzgiris and Hunt tasks (Pepperberg & Kozak, 1986); here we wanted to track Griffin from a very early age to see if his development paralleled that of children (it did, but he progressed more quickly)—plus, our concern was that our parrots might be smart enough to track something of interest, but not necessarily remember what it was that they were tracking.

One way to check this possibility was to do the ‘treat substitution’ trick. Other researchers (e.g., Tinklepaugh, 1928, 1932) had examined how nonhuman primates reacted to being tricked with “reat substitution,” so we thought that would be an interesting task for our birds. And they reacted similarly—Griffin turned over the cup under which he thought he would find a nut, didn’t find the expected cashew, then turned over all the other cups and, when that didn’t work, ran over to the experimenters!

Alex reacted similarly, but also stared at the experimenters with slitted eyes—an aggressive stance—and added some emphatic banging of his beak on the table, which for him was a signal of displeasure and frustration. At the time we did these experiments, we couldn’t videotape each trial with a tiny camera or a mobile phone as we do now (the experiment was actually done in 1996!), so we don’t have a cinematic record—just our notes (thus no pretty pictures this month).

Of course, this behavior makes a lot of sense when one thinks about the lives of nonhumans that are usually preyed upon in the wild—they form expectations of where food should be over time, and if it is not there, they will have wasted a lot of precious foraging time and effort and possibly exposed themselves to predators for naught. And, of course, if they take the time to break open a particular type of nutshell, they definitely expect to find that type of nut! In general, novelty in the wild is more likely to indicate some form of danger (e.g., an unexpected shadow could be a hawk looking for a meal) rather than something fun, which is why most parrots are somewhat neophobic (i.e., shy away from new objects or situations, and dislike surprises).

Thus, although these findings in parrots and jays may not have violated our expectations, as scientists we are always interested in testing our hypotheses. And we are always excited when we find data to show that those hypotheses were correct!

References

Pepperberg, I.M. & Kozak, F.A. (1986). Object permanence in the African Grey parrot (Psittacus erithacus). Anim. Learn. Behav. 14:322‑330.

Pepperberg, I.M., Willner, M.R., & Gravitz, L.B. (1997). Development of Piagetian object permanence in a Grey parrot (Psittacus erithacus). J. Comp. Psychol. 111:63–75.

Piaget, J. (1952). The origins of intelligence in children (M. Cook, Trans.). New York: International Universities Press. (Original work published 1936)

Schnell, A.K., Maria Loconsole, M., Garcia-Pelegrin, E., Clive Wilkins, C., & Nicola S. Clayton, N.S. (2021). Jays are sensitive to cognitive illusions. R. Soc. Open Sci. 8: 202358. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsos.202358

Tinklepaugh, O. T. (1928). An experimental study of representative factors in monkeys. J. Comp. Psychol. 8:197-236.

Tinklepaugh, O. T. (1932). Multiple delayed reaction with chimpanzees and monkeys. J. Comp. Psychol. 13:207-243.

Uzgiris, I. C., & Hunt, J. (1975). Assessment in infancy: Ordinal scales of psychological development. Champaign: University of Illinois Press.

Products You May Like

Articles You May Like

Kittens Brought in at 2 Weeks Old for New Start, Now Have So Much Energy They Even Impress Family Dog
13 Dog Breeds Who Are Incredibly Social
The Dracula Parrot—The Truth Behind Its Spooky Name
They Take in a Cat with Kittens and Notice She Begins Following Other Felines, Gaining Courage
Are Christmas Trees Toxic to Dogs?

1 Comment

  1. Pingback: bad links

Leave a Reply

Your email address will not be published. Required fields are marked *